Posted December 4, 2005
Food for Thought on Homosexuality
Homosexuality:
The Essentialist Argument
Continues to Erode
By A. Dean Byrd, Ph.D., MBA, MPH
The essentialist argument that homosexuality is biologically determined, and
is therefore not amenable to change, continues to find little support in
science. Finding its way in to the Monitor on Psychology, the official
magazine of the American Psychological Association, another study emphasizes
the fluidity of homosexual attraction.
Dr. Ellen Scheter of the Fielding Graduate Institute presented her research
at the recent meeting of the American Psychological Association. Her
qualitative study included in-depth interviews with 11 women who had been
self-identified as lesbian for more than 10 years. All of these women were
in heterosexual relationships which had been ongoing for more than a year
(Greer, 2004).
These findings provide support for the research of Dr. Lisa Diamond whose
study was reported in the Monitor on Psychology in 2000. Dr. Diamond
concluded that "sexual identity was far from fixed in women who aren't
exclusively heterosexual" (Murray, 2000, p. 15; Diamond, 2000).
Dr. Kenneth Zucker, in his careful analysis of the innate/immutable argument
of homosexuality, rostered a plethora of studies to support his conclusion
that "sexual orientation is more fluid than fixed" (2003, pp. 399-400).
Friedman and Downey, the psychiatric researchers at Columbia University,
offered a strongly worded conclusion opposing the essentialist argument: "At
clinical conferences one often hears...that homosexual orientation is fixed
and unmodifiable. Neither assertion is true...The assertion that
homosexuality is genetic is so reductionistic that it must be dismissed out
of hand as a general principle of psychology" (2002, p 39).
Yet the national organizations continue to offer the essentialist argument
as a guide for law and public policy. No reputable scientist on either side
of the political spectrum would disagree with the conclusion of Friedman and
Downey. Even the gay-activist researchers themselves who studies have been
used by the media to trumpet the message that homosexuality is biologically
determined do not support the "born that way" myth.
Simon LeVay, the author of the hypothalamus study, noted, "It's important to
stress what I didn't find. I did not prove that homosexuality was genetic,
or find a genetic cause for being gay. I didn't show that gay men were born
that way, the most common mistake people make in interpreting my work"
(Nimmons, 1994, p. 64).
Dean Hamer, the author of the "gay gene" study, agreed: "We knew that genes
were only part of the answer. We assumed the environment also played a role
in sexual orientation, as it does in most, if not all behaviors...(Hamer and
Copeland, 1994, p. 82). Hamer further emphasizes, "Homosexuality is not
purely genetic . . . environmental factors play a role. There is not a single
master gene that makes people gay . . . I don't think we will ever predict who
will be gay" (Mitchell, 1995).
LeVay, the gay activist researcher, made an interesting observation about
the emphasis on the biology of homosexuality: He noted, "...people who think
that gays and lesbians are born that way are also more likely to support gay
rights" (1996, p. 282).
Psychiatrist Robert Spitzer, a prominent member of the American Psychiatric
Association and historic ally of gay activists, provides even more insight
into how activism has replaced science in the national organizations. He
notes, "There's a gay-activist group that's very strong and very vocal and
recognized officially by the American Psychiatric Association. There is
nobody to give the other viewpoint...There may be a few people...but they
don't talk" (Spitzer, 2004).
May be it is time for legislators to call into question the "science" of the
national organizations, particularly when the national organizations employ
activism disguised as science to justify resolutions and policy statements.
References
Diamond, L. M. (2000). Sexual identity, attractions, and behavior among
young sexual minority women over a 2 year period. Developmental Psychology,
36 (2), pp. 241-250. Friedman, R. C. & Downey, J.I. (2002). Sexual
orientation and psychoanalysis: sexual science and clinical practice (New
York: Columbia University Press). p. 39.
Greer, M. (2004). Labels may oversimplify women's sexual identity,
experiences. Monitor on Psychology, 35, 9, p. 28.
______, (2004). I Do Exist (video). Robert Spitzer Interview.
Hamer, D. & Copeland, P. (1994). The science of desire. New York: Simon &
Schuster.
LeVay, S. (1996). Queer science. Cambridge, MIT Press.
Mitchell, N, (1995). Genetics, sexuality linked, study says. Standard
Examiner, April 30.
Murray, B. (2000). Sexual identity is far from fixed in women who aren't
exclusively heterosexual. Monitor on Psychology, 32(3), pp. 64-67.
Nimmons, D. (1994). Sexual brain. Discover, 5, 3.
Zucker, K. J. (2003). The politics and science of reparative therapy.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, pp. 399-400.
|